Slow-wave sleep ( SWS), often referred to as deep sleep, is the third stage of non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM), where electroencephalography activity is characterised by slow Delta wave.
Slow-wave sleep usually lasts between 70 and 90 minutes, taking place during the first hours of the night. Slow-wave sleep is characterised by moderate muscle tone, slow or absent eye movement, and lack of genital activity. Slow-wave sleep is considered important for memory consolidation, Explicit memory, and the recovery of the brain from daily activities.
Before 2007, the term slow-wave sleep referred to the third and fourth stages of NREM. Current terminology combined these into a single stage three.
Before 2007, the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) divided slow-wave sleep into stages 3 and 4.Iber, C; Ancoli-Israel, S; Chesson, A; Quan, SF. for the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007. The two stages are now combined as Stage Three or N3. An epoch (30 seconds of sleep) that consists of 20% or more slow-wave (delta) sleep is now considered slow-wave sleep.
Specifically, SWS presents a role in spatial declarative memory. Reactivation of the hippocampus during SWS is detected after the spatial learning task. In addition, a correlation can be observed between the amplitude of hippocampal activity during SWS and the improvement in spatial memory performance, such as route retrieval, on the following day. Additionally, studies have found that when odour cues are given to subjects during sleep, this stage of sleep exclusively allows contextual cues to be reactivated after sleep, favoring their consolidation. A separate study found that when subjects hear sounds associated with previously shown pictures of locations, the reactivation of individual memory representations was significantly higher during SWS as compared to other sleep stages.
Affective representations are generally better remembered during sleep compared to neutral ones. Emotions with negative salience presented as a cue during SWS show better reactivation, and therefore an enhanced consolidation in comparison to neutral memories. The former was predicted by sleep spindles over SWS, which discriminates the memory processes during sleep as well as facilitating emotional memory consolidation. Acetylcholine plays an essential role in hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. An increased level of cholinergic activity during SWS is known to be disruptive to memory processing. Considering that acetylcholine is a neurotransmitter that modulates the direction of information flow between the hippocampus and neocortex during sleep, its suppression is necessary during SWS to consolidate sleep-related declarative memory.
Sleep deprivation studies with humans suggest that the primary function of slow-wave sleep may be to allow the brain to recover from its daily activities. Glucose metabolism in the brain increases as a result of tasks that demand mental activity. Another function affected by slow-wave sleep is the secretion of growth hormone, which is always greatest during this stage.
Longer periods of SWS occur in the first part of the night, primarily in the first two sleep cycles (roughly three hours). Children and young adults will have more total SWS in a night than older adults. The elderly may not go into SWS at all during many nights of sleep.
NREM sleep, as observed on the electroencephalogram (EEG), is distinguished by certain characteristic features. Sleep spindles, marked by spindle-like changes in the amplitude of 12–14 Hz oscillations, K complexes lasting at least 0.5 seconds, consisting of a distinct negative sharp wave followed by a positive component, and slow waves or delta waves characterized by slow frequency (< 2 Hz) and high amplitude (> 75 μV) are key indicators. The presence and distribution of sleep spindle activity and slow waves vary across NREM sleep, leading to its subdivision into stages 1–3. While slow waves and sleep spindles are present in stages 2 and 3, stage 2 sleep is characterized by a higher prevalence of spindles, while slow waves dominate the EEG during stage 3.
Slow-wave sleep is an active phenomenon probably brought about by the activation of Serotonin neurons of the Raphe nuclei system.
The slow wave seen in the cortical EEG is generated through recurrent connections within the cerebral cortex, where cortical pyramidal cells excite one another in a positive feedback loop. This recurrent excitation is balanced by inhibition, resulting in the active state of the slow oscillation of slow wave sleep. Failure of this mechanism results in a silencing of activity for a brief period. The recurrence of active and silent periods occurs at a rate of 0.5–4 Hz, giving rise to the slow waves of the EEG seen during slow wave sleep.
Considering that SWS is the only sleep stage that reports human deep sleep as well as being used in studies with mammals and birds, it is also adopted in experiments revealing the role of hemispheric asymmetries during sleep. A predominance of the left hemisphere in the neural activity can be observed in the default-mode network during SWS. This asymmetry is correlated with the sleep onset latency, which is a sensitive parameter of the so-called first night effect—the reduced quality of sleep during the first session in the laboratory.
The left hemisphere is shown to be more sensitive to deviant stimuli during the first night—compared to the following nights of an experiment. This asymmetry explains further the reduced sleep of half the brain during SWS. Indeed, in comparison to the right one, the left hemisphere plays a vigilant role during SWS.
Furthermore, a faster behavioral reactivity is detected in the left hemisphere during SWS of the first night. The rapid awakening is correlated to the regional asymmetry in the activities of SWS. These findings show that the hemispheric asymmetry in SWS plays a role as a protective mechanism. SWS is therefore sensitive to danger and a non-familiar environment, creating a need for vigilance and reactivity during sleep.
When sleep-deprived humans sleep normally again, the recovery percentage for each stage of sleep is not the same. Only seven percent of stages one and two are regained, but 68 percent of stage-four slow-wave sleep and 53 percent of REM sleep are regained. This suggests that stage-four sleep (known today as the deepest part of stage-three sleep) is more important than the other stages.
During slow-wave sleep, there is a significant decline in cerebral metabolic rate and cerebral blood flow. The activity falls to about 75 percent of the normal wakefulness level. The regions of the brain that are most active when awake have the highest level of delta waves during slow-wave sleep. This indicates that the rest is geographical. The "shutting down" of the brain accounts for the grogginess and confusion if someone is awakened during deep sleep since it takes the cerebral cortex time to resume its normal functions.
According to J. Siegel (2005), sleep deprivation results in the build-up of free radicals and in the brain. Free radicals are oxidizing agents that have one unpaired electron, making them highly reactive. These free radicals interact with electrons of biomolecules and damage cells. In slow-wave sleep, the decreased rate of metabolism reduces the creation of oxygen byproducts, thereby allowing the existing radical species to clear. This is a means of preventing damage to the brain.Carlson, Neil R. (2012). Physiology of Behavior. Pearson. p. 299-300. .
Moreover, the onset of Alzheimer's disease is marked by the deposition of amyloid beta (Aβ) in the brain. AD is distinguished by the presence of amyloid-beta plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. These structural anomalies are linked to disruptions in the sleep-wake cycle, particularly in non-REM slow wave sleep. Thus, individuals diagnosed with Alzheimer's often experience disturbances in sleep, resulting in diminished levels of non-rapid eye movement sleep and reduced slow wave activity, that is a prominent brain rhythm during deep non-REM sleep. Similarly, even cognitively healthy individuals with detectable amyloid beta exhibit sleep disturbances, characterized by compromised sleep quality and an increased frequency of daytime napping.
Slow-wave sleep and slow-wave activity undergo significant transformations throughout one's lifespan, with aging serving as a particularly influential factor in predicting individual variations. Aging is inversely proportional to the amount of SWS beginning by midlife, so slow-wave sleep declines with age. Moreover, recent findings indicate that older individuals exhibit a decreased inclination for daytime sleep compared to younger counterparts, and this decline persists even when accounting for variations in habitual sleep duration. This age-related decrease in daytime sleep propensity is evident in middle-aged individuals and coincides with statistically significant reductions in total sleep time, slow-wave sleep, and slow-wave activity.
Sex differences have also been found, such that females tend to have higher levels of slow-wave sleep than males, at least up until menopause. Older individuals exhibit gender-based variations in non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, where women demonstrate increased slow-wave sleep during both regular and recuperative sleep. There have also been studies that have shown differences between races. The results showed that there was a lower percentage of slow-wave sleep in African Americans compared to Caucasians, but since there are many influencing factors (e.g., body mass index, sleep-disordered breathing, obesity, diabetes, and hypertension), this potential difference must be investigated further.
Mental disorders play a role in individual differences in the quality and quantity of slow-wave sleep: subjects with depression show a lower amplitude of slow-wave activity compared to healthy participants. Sex differences also persist in the former group: depressed men present significantly lower slow-wave amplitude. This sex divergence is twice as large as the one observed in healthy subjects. However, no age-related difference concerning slow-wave sleep can be observed in the depressed group.
Some of the brain regions implicated in the induction of slow-wave sleep include:
Gamma-hydroxybutyrate is synthesized in the central nervous system from gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA). Oral administration has been shown to enhance slow-wave sleep without suppressing REM sleep. In the United States, it is sold as a prescription drug under the brand name Xyrem. It has been shown to reduce cataplexy attacks and excessive daytime sleepiness in patients with narcolepsy.
The administration of the GABAa agonist Gaboxadol enhances both deep sleep and also positively impacts various indicators of insomnia.
Tiagabine, a selective GABA reuptake inhibitor, demonstrated improving sleep maintenance and significantly increasing SWS in healthy elderly subjects and adult patients with primary insomnia.
Levodopa is a drug commonly used to treat Parkinson's disease which acts to increases the brain's dopamine availability. Nocturnal single doses of levodopa increase slow-wave sleep by 10.6% in the elderly.
Antagonists of certain serotonergic receptors (namely 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C) have also been demonstrated to enhance slow-wave sleep, although they do not consistently bring about improvements in overall sleep duration or symptoms associated with insomnia. Trazodone, an atypical antidepressant, increases the duration of low-wave sleep; it is suspected that trazodone's antagonistic action at the 5-HT2A receptor may contribute to this effect. A variety of drugs that antagonise the on 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors exhibit SWS-enhancing effects in humans.
|
|